Too much practicing?

I am always fascinated by explanations of neural functions in the brain while practicing.
I loved this article regarding how much practice is too much.

Thinking of a musical performance in terms of tennis match or basketball game
is nothing new, thanks to the wildly popular (at least among musicians)
Inner Game of Tennis/Music.

This gave me a fresh perspective about something I have been mulling over recently,
namely my frustration about my inability to react to individual moments within an audition.
I have spent the year facing my performance anxiety head-on.
Now that I have cleared the cobwebs of being blinded by my fear,
I would love to feel in control, not just mechanical.
And this is the perfect reason to stick with it.

So, back to those excerpts...



More practice cuts down on unnecessary movements and eliminates wasted energy, 
a new study shows.
By Annie Murphy Paul
Why do I have to keep practicing? I know it already!”
That’s the familiar wail of a child seated at the piano or in front of the multiplication table (or, for that matter, of an adult taking a tennis lesson). Cognitive science has a persuasive retort: We don’t just need to learn a task in order to perform it well; we need to overlearn it. Decades of research have shown that superior performance requires practicing beyond the point of mastery. The perfect execution of a piano sonata or a tennis serve doesn’t mark the end of practice; it signals that the crucial part of the session is just getting underway.

New evidence of why this is so was provided by a study published in the Journal of Neuroscience earlier this month. Assistant professor Alaa Ahmed and two of her colleagues in the integrative physiology department at the University of Colorado-Boulder asked study subjects to move a cursor on a screen by manipulating a robotic arm. As they did so, the researchers measured the participants’ energy expenditure by analyzing how much oxygen they inhaled and how much carbon dioxide they breathed out. When the subjects first tackled the exercise, they used up a lot of metabolic power, but this decreased as their skill improved. By the end of the learning process, the amount of effort they expended to carry out the task had declined about 20 percent from when they started.
“The lesson here is keep on practicing, even after it seems the task has been learned.”
Whenever we learn to make a new movement, Ahmed explains, we form and then update an internal model—a “sensorimotor map”—which our nervous system uses to predict our muscles’ motions and the resistance they will encounter. As that internal model is refined over time, we’re able to cut down on unnecessary movements and eliminate wasted energy.
Over the course of a practice session, the subjects in Ahmed’s study were becoming more efficient in their muscle activity. But that wasn’t the whole story. Energy expenditures continued to decrease even after the decline in muscle activity had stabilized. In fact, Ahmed and her coauthors report, this is when the greatest reductions in metabolic power were observed—during the very time when it looks to an observer, and to the participant herself, as if “nothing is happening.”
What’s going on here? Ahmed theorizes that even after participants had fine-tuned their muscle movements, the neural processes controlling the movements continued to grow more efficient. The brain uses up energy, too, and through overlearning it can get by on less. These gains in mental efficiency free up resources for other tasks: infusing the music you’re playing with greater emotion and passion, for example, or keeping closer track of your opponent’s moves on the other side of the tennis court. Less effort in one domain means more energy available to others.
While Ahmed’s paper didn’t address the application of overlearning to the classroom, other studies have demonstrated that for a wide variety of academic activities–from recalling vocabulary words to solving math problems–overlearning reduces the amount of effort required to carry out the job at hand.
“The message from this study is that in order to perform with less effort, keep on practicing, even after it seems the task has been learned,” says Ahmed. “We have shown there is an advantage to continued practice beyond any visible changes in performance.” In other words: You’re getting better and better, even when you can’t tell you’re improving—a thought to keep you going through those long hours of practice.
Annie Murphy Paul, the author of Origins, is at work on a book about the science of learning.

0 comments:

Post a Comment